Jump to content

User:RagingLaxatives/sandbox

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Nalini Ambady
Nalini Ambady in 2009
Born(1959-03-20)March 20, 1959
DiedOctober 28, 2013(2013-10-28) (aged 54)
Alma materHarvard University
College of William and Mary
Known forthin slice judgments
AwardsAAAS Prize for Behavioral Science Research
Scientific career
FieldsPsychology
InstitutionsStanford University
Tufts University
Harvard University
Doctoral advisorRobert Rosenthal

Nalini Ambady (March 20, 1959 – October 28, 2013) was an Indian-American social psychologist and a leading expert on nonverbal behavior and interpersonal perception.[1] She introduced and conducted extensive research around the concept of thin slice judgments. She was a Professor at Stanford University in the Department of Psychology, and subsequently was a founder of the SPARQ center. Her death from leukemia sparked campaigns to increase the number of South-Asian bone marrow registries globally.

Early life and education

[edit]

A native of the state of Kerala, India, Ambady did her schooling at the Lawrence School, Lovedale, and joined college at the Lady Shri Ram College for Women, Delhi. Subsequently, she moved to the United States for higher education, completing her M.A. in Psychology from The College of William and Mary, Virginia. She earned her Ph.D. in social psychology from Harvard University in 1991 under the guidance of Robert Rosenthal, with whom she researched thin slice judgments.[2][3]

Academic career

[edit]

She held academic positions at Harvard University, Cambridge and the College of the Holy Cross, Massachusetts before being appointed as Professor in the Department of Psychology at Tufts University in 2004.[4] She subsequently moved to Stanford University, California in 2011. She was the first Indian-American woman to teach psychology at Harvard, Tufts, and Stanford.

Ambady specialized in the study of intuition. Her research found that humans perceive nonverbal cues in response to novel people or situations and that the information gleaned from an instant impression is often as powerful as information gleaned by getting to know a situation or person over a longer period of time.[5] She and Robert Rosenthal coined the term "thin slices" to refer to such instantaneous non-verbal cues. Later, author Malcolm Gladwell referred extensively to Ambady's work in his popular book, Blink: The Power of Thinking Without Thinking.

One of Ambady's more well-known experiments asked students to watch silent 10-second videos of unfamiliar professors as they taught, and to rate the professors for likability, honesty, competence, and other qualities. The students' responses correlated remarkably well with similar ratings by students who had spent a full semester getting to know the professors' personalities and teaching qualities.[5]

Founding of SPARQ
[edit]

During Ambady's appointment at Stanford, she founded SPARQ, the Center for Social Psychological Answers to Real-World Questions. The center was initially called "The Lewin Center", after social psychology pioneer Kurt Lewin. SPARQ officially opened its doors in 2014 after the passing of Ambady, however, she remained active in its formation even during her struggle with leukemia, and until her death.[6] The main goal of SPARQ is to improve society by taking knowledge from the field of social psychology directly to policymakers, teachers and other impactful societal figures. SPARQ is an attempt to address the gap between psychology and the real world and aims to build a bridge between the hands-on experiences of practitioners in the field, and the scientific findings of the lab. SPARQ attempts to accomplish this goal through the fostering of meaningful collaborations between practitioners and social psychologies to the benefit of both.[7][8]

Struggle with Leukemia

[edit]

Ambady was diagnosed with leukemia in May 2004 but recovered after treatment. In 2011, the cancer recurred in a more aggressive form.[9] Her friends and family led an intensive worldwide campaign to find a compatible bone-marrow donor since they were unable to successfully locate any in existing bone-marrow registries. This was partly due to the low numbers of Indians on such registries worldwide and a limited base of donors numbering around 25,000 in the few Indian registries that exist.[10][11] Her plight sparked a global effort to increase participation in bone marrow registries among South Asian ethnic groups.[12] Though as many as thirteen potential donors were located over a period of time, many of them refused to go through with the transplant process after identification.[9]

Ambady died on October 28, 2013 at Brigham and Women's Hospital in Boston.[5][13]


Research

[edit]

Culture and the brain

[edit]

Nalini Ambady and Jamshed Bharucha collaborated on a study published in the journal 'Current Directions in Psychological Science' in 2009 that focused on articles regarding how culture can affect neural activation. They suggest a framework for cultural neuroscience in which both features are objective: culture mapping and source analysis.[14] Culture mapping is a research technique that requires mapping cognitive or neural differences across cultures. It also involves regulating which cognitive or neural processes differ across cultures without determining whether differences are learned or innate.[14]

Culture mapping can be in two forms. The first form can show how the environments of different cultures are processed inversely by individuals from a particular culture. The second form illustrates how the same environment is processed differently from distinctive cultures. Ambady included a study conducted by Tang et al (2006) which looked at the brain activity of native English and native Chinese speakers while performing numerical and non-numerical tasks.[15] Their results showed that native English speakers had more activation in the left perisylvian cortices such as Wernickesand Broca's area, which are connected with language. However, Native Chinese speakers demonstrated more activation in a visual premotor association area, which is correlated with visual-spatial processing and functions tied to cognitive functioning during numerical tasks. [14]

Moreover, Ambady discusses source analysis as the process of defining the source or causes of culture mappings. She explains three sources of cultural universals and differences: genetic commonality or difference, cultural learning meditated by brain plasticity and the degree of similarity between cultural environments. The first source is genetic commonality or difference. Behavioural genetics studies examining the functional role of 5-HTT have recognized individuals carrying the short allele seem prone to higher levels of anxiety and depression compared to long allele carriers. In addition, individuals with the short allele showed greater amygdala activation, during an emotion-matching task relative to individuals with the long allele.[14]

The second source is cultural learning and exposure meditated by brain plasticity. The evidence shows how cultural learning originates from inspecting neural processing in bilinguals and learners of a second language. A study included in the article compared Korean adults who were adopted by French families before the age of 8. The results demonstrate how Korean-born subjects did not remember their native languages and were fluent in French. However, both groups demonstrated parallel patterns of activation to sentences spoken in French, Korean or other foreign languages, implying that proficiency in the language of the new culture was connected with neural changes.[14]

Finally, similarity or difference explains the difference in stimuli and pattern structures across cultures. In a meta-analysis comparison, the neuroimaging results for word reading across different languages and cultures found one common area of dispensation across language and cultures. They proposed the existence of a visual word form area in the left mid fusiform gyrus, which seems to be central to word recognition. They also found culture and language-specific patterns of activation. This was through the example of the Chinese writing system which, for example, requires more activation in the visual areas in both hemispheres than do western alphabets.[14]

Overall, Ambady's study shows the neuroscience of culture is at a promising stage. Recent studies and research in the field of cultural differences are sparser than cultural mapping and source analysis research, but new evidence promises to advance this.

Racial Bias and Stereotyping

[edit]

Race salience and essentialist thinking in racial stereotype development

Nalini Ambady conducted a vast number of research to understand how and why individuals develop racial biases and why they stereotype others.

Nalini Ambady, Kristin Pauker, and Evan P. Apfelbaum, collaborated on a research study that investigated the arrival and the background of racial stereotyping by studying 89 children between the ages of 3 and 10. For the study, the children had to complete a variety of tasks related to matching and sorting.[16] These tasks were used as a measure to assess the children’s knowledge and how they apply positive and negative stereotypes when encountered with in-groups and out-groups. The results from those tasks implied that children start to use stereotypes on out-groups when they are around 6 years old. This could occur based on a number of factors, but this study showed that there were 2 predictors that played an important contribution to the use of these stereotypes. The first predictor was race salience, which is where one sees and then organizes those people by race. The second predictor is essentialist thinking, which is where one believes that a certain race cannot change. These two predictors showed when and how these racial stereotypes may be taking place in real-life situations.[17]


Stereotype susceptibility in children: Effects of identity activation on quantitative performance

Nalini Ambady, Margaret Shih, Amy Kim, and Todd L. Pittinsky collaborated on a research study that examined the impact of positive and negative stereotypes had on cognitive performance.[18] They accomplished this using two studies which consisted of three separate age groups of children. The first group was between the grade of kindergarten to grade 2 (lower elementary grades). The second group was between the grade of grade 3 and grade 5 (upper elementary grades). The third group was between the grades of grade 6 and grade 8 (middle school). The study showed results of children in lower elementary grades and middle school grades having shifts in their performance based on the positive and negative stereotypes. These findings were also consistent with the adult findings. The stereotypes were subtle and resulted in negative stereotypes impeding one’s performance and positive stereotypes facilitating one’s performance. Stereotype susceptibility development is a critical area and is a necessity to be able to understand the connections between an individual’s behavior and stereotypes.[19]

In a different study conducted by Ambady and her colleagues, she looked at the effects of stereotype saliency on the performance of minorities, specifically in this case Asian Women. Because of the different stereotypes associated with the identities of Asian women; Ambady hypothesized that by making one identity salient, in this case, either gender or race, it would impact the women's performance because it would make different stereotypes salient. What Ambady found was, the women who had their gender made salient, performed poorly on mathematical tasks when compared to control. This was in-line with the stereotype that women are less proficient in math. When the women had their Asian identities made salient, their performance improved relative to control; reflecting the stereotype of Asians being "good at math". These findings were significant as they showed a causal link between stereotype saliency and one's performance, which has real-world impacts.[19]

Nonverbal Behavior

[edit]

Nonverbal communication and psychology: Past and future

[edit]

Ambady and Marvin A. Hecht investigated the relationship between the study of nonverbal communication and psychology. The study of nonverbal communication was invented in the 1950s primarily as a cross-disciplinary effort on the part of the psychiatrists, linguists and anthropologists. This was surveyed in the 1960s and 1970s by an explosion of empirical research, books, and popular media attention. However, in the 1908s psychologists began to frequently integrate nonverbal communication variables into new research. Attention to nonverbal cues waned as the cognitive revolution gained momentum. In this generation, there is a renaissance of awareness in nonverbal communication, particularly among those who study emotion, psychophysiology and person perception. Consequently, the future of nonverbal communication may lie where it started; as an interdisciplinary venture.[20]

Unspoken cultural influence: Exposure to and influence of nonverbal bias

[edit]

In a research study published in 2009, Nalini Ambady and Max Weisbuch explored the importance of nonverbal behavior in shaping culture. They used television as a communication medium to investigate the impacts of nonverbal bias on individuals. They discovered that nonverbal bias influenced individuals’ beliefs, attitudes, and behavior. In research constituting 4 studies, they found out that there was a favorable nonverbal attitude towards especially slim female characters in popular TV programs, and exposure to nonverbal bias against heavier women in TV programs influenced the participants’ attitudes about ideal weight. In the last study, they found out that the regional difference in exposure to nonverbal bias accounted for regional variance in unhealthy dieting behavior among female teenagers.[21]

Misery Loves Company: When Sadness Increases the Desire for Social Connectedness

[edit]

In an attempt to specify the potential functions of sadness, Heather M. Gray, Keiko Ishii, and Nalini Ambady examined the effects of induced sadness on attention and motivation.[22] Functional theories hold that different emotions elicit different adaptive responses in the mind and body to prepare us for specific events or surrounding environment. Within this study, Nalini Ambady and colleagues draw upon the previous research by Pickett and colleagues based on the existence of social monitoring systems (SMS). Nalini and colleagues conducted three experiments to investigate the effects of sadness on attention motivation: In the 1st experiment, they built upon the previous research showing that individuals concerned with social connectedness, were specifically attuned to the vocal tone of speech as an important nonverbal social cue. Nalini and colleagues discovered that sadness associated with social loss resulted in attentional bias towards vocal tone. In their second study, they found a significant effect of induced sadness on increased and urgent motivations for affiliation with others. In the third and last part of their study, they provided evidence to distinguish the effects of social loss sadness versus status loss sadness. In contrary to hedonic contingency hypothesis, their result showed that sad individuals were selective in their choice of behavior; the results established in the 1st and 2nd parts of the study concerning attentional bias toward vocal tone and increased desire for social affiliation were restricted to sadness precipitated by social loss, whereas sadness as a result of status loss did not have the same effects.[23]

"Thin slices" of behavior

[edit]

In collaboration with Kathleen R. Bogart and Michael L. Slepian, Nalini advanced the study of “thin slices” of behavior when researching the ability to precisely recognize characteristic traits people possess along with their individuality. This method establishes a way to predict an individual’s behavior with something as simple as a swift observation of their nonverbal behavior.[24]

With a glimpse of someone’s “thin slices” of behavior, Nalini and her colleagues believed that just by observing nonverbal behavior, one can determine with accuracy to a certain extent, behavioral outcomes influenced by personality disorders (e.g. anxiety, depression etc.); mental states (e.g. suicide); and how an individual adapts to life events. One could even go as far as specific types of life events such as traumatic experience (e.g. divorce, sexual abuse etc.). [25]

There is only one limitation to this method, a clinician’s analysis can falter when presented with a patient suffering from particular clinical disabilities (e.g. movement disorders such as Parkinson’s disease), since healthy nonverbal behavior is impaired or masked by the exhibited symptoms of these disorders. Conventional methods already entail clinicians to observe their patients and make diagnoses through intuition based on the patient’s personality traits and nonverbal behavior. With the added method of “thin-slice” judgment, specialists would have another valuable resource to provide more qualitative assessments of their patients.[25]  

Accuracy of prediction
[edit]

Previously, through a meta-analysis published in 1992,  Nalini and co-author Rober Rosenthal coined the term “thin slices” of expressive behavior as a valid method of predicting human behavior. The results of their findings presents that thin slices provide a 70% accuracy prediction as opposed to the 30% when using other assessment methods.[26]

They provided three explanations as to why this method is so accurate:

Emotions such as anger, fear or dominance are easily recognized due to how well they are recognized universally and the important role they play in survival and adaptability. In contrast, non-crucial survival emotions such as humor, are more challenging to detect due to the necessity to identify through inferential processes.[27]

  • Truths to stereotypes and their effects on self-fulfilling prophecies.

It is believed that individuals react to stereotype cues, whether they are biological or behavioral. These stereotypes create expectations in others that influence an individual’s behavior toward the target person and/or group. Research has demonstrated how such expectations will affect people’s positive or negative behavior toward targeted individuals. By exhibiting these behaviors toward target individuals, they, in turn, modify their behavior and confirm the previous expectations. This creates an endless loop of self-fulfilling prophecies.[27]

  • Evidence based on the disruptive effect of thinking and reasoning.

A reason why evidence drawn from “thin slices” is accurate is due to the fact that the individuals were not exposed to distracting stimuli. Research found that subjects were less accurate in judgement toward other target individuals during face-to-face scenarios in contrast to impressions evaluated from videotaped targeted individuals. This draws upon two types of stimuli, external and internal.[27]

External stimuli refer to verbal components that can be distracting within actual interactions and require cognitive resources to process internally. These internal processes can also be distracting because it requires the function of thinking and reasoning which can overpower and cloud judgment. Thin slices of behavior are "snap" judgments, therefore, bypass all the distracting stimuli, creating a more accurate impression of the targeted individual(s).[27]


Awards and Honours

[edit]

Dr. Ambady was a fellow of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, the American Psychological Association, and the Association for Psychological Science. She won the AAAS Prize for Behavioral Science Research in 1993.[28] She was presented the Carol and Ed Diener Award in Social Psychology in 1999 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology for substantial contribution to the field.[8] She also received the Presidential Early Career Award for Scientists and Engineers from President Bill Clinton.[5]


Books

[edit]

Notes and References

[edit]
  1. ^ Gladwell, Malcolm, 1963- (2005). Blink : the power of thinking without thinking (1st ed ed.). New York: Little, Brown and Co. ISBN 0316172324. OCLC 55679231. {{cite book}}: |edition= has extra text (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  2. ^ Ambady, Nalini; Rosenthal, Robert (1992). "Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 111 (2): 256–274. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.256. ISSN 1939-1455.
  3. ^ Tannenbaum, Melanie. "Rest In Peace, Nalini Ambady". Scientific American. Retrieved 30 October 2013.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: url-status (link)
  4. ^ Ambady, Nalini. "Curriculum Vitae" (PDF). Tufts University. Retrieved 30 October 2013.
  5. ^ a b c d Fox, Margalit (4 November 2013). "Nalini Ambady, Psychologist of Intuition, Is Dead at 54". The New York Times. Retrieved 12 November 2013.
  6. ^ Conner, Alana C. (2014-02-28). "Stanford SPARQ Sparks Change". APS Observer. 27 (3).
  7. ^ "Ambady leaves behind legacy of influential research, respect for her students". Stanford Daily. 8 November 2013. Retrieved 14 January 2014.
  8. ^ a b "Nalini Ambady, Stanford psychology professor, dies at 54". Stanford News. 31 October 2013. Archived from the original on 4 December 2013. Retrieved 14 January 2014. Cite error: The named reference "SR" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  9. ^ a b D'Souza, Dilip. "Will We Save Nalini Ambady?". Yahoo. Retrieved 30 October 2013.
  10. ^ Efforts on to find bone marrow match for Nalini, Times of India, 2013, retrieved 2013-05-07
  11. ^ Teo, Niuniu (2013), Students organize to find bone marrow match for Prof. Nalini Ambady, Stanford, CA: Stanford University, retrieved 2013-05-07
  12. ^ Gaufberg, Liz (May 2013). "Saving Nalini: Leading Psychologist Seeks Bone Marrow Donor to Survive". WBUR.
  13. ^ "Bone marrow search fails, Stanford professor Nalini Ambady dies in US". Deccan Chronicle. 30 October 2013. Retrieved 30 October 2013.
  14. ^ a b c d e f Ambady, Nalini; Bharucha, Jamshed (2009-12). "Culture and the Brain". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 18 (6): 342–345. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01664.x. ISSN 0963-7214. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  15. ^ Tang, Yiyuan; Zhang, Wutian; Chen, Kewei; Feng, Shigang; Ji, Ye; Shen, Junxian; Reiman, Eric M.; Liu, Yijun (2006). "Arithmetic Processing in the Brain Shaped by Cultures". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 103 (28): 10775–10780. ISSN 0027-8424.
  16. ^ Pauker, Kristin; Ambady, Nalini; Apfelbaum, Evan P. (2010-11). "Race Salience and Essentialist Thinking in Racial Stereotype Development: Racial Stereotype Development". Child Development. 81 (6): 1799–1813. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01511.x. PMC 3052875. PMID 21077865. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  17. ^ Pauker, Kristin; Ambady, Nalini; Apfelbaum, Evan P. (2010-11). "Race Salience and Essentialist Thinking in Racial Stereotype Development: Racial Stereotype Development". Child Development. 81 (6): 1799–1813. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01511.x. PMC 3052875. PMID 21077865. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  18. ^ Ambady, Nalini; Shih, Margaret; Kim, Amy; Pittinsky, Todd L. (2001-9). "Stereotype Susceptibility in Children: Effects of Identity Activation on Quantitative Performance". Psychological Science. 12 (5): 385–390. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00371. ISSN 0956-7976. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  19. ^ a b Ambady, Nalini; Shih, Margaret; Kim, Amy; Pittinsky, Todd L. (2001-9). "Stereotype Susceptibility in Children: Effects of Identity Activation on Quantitative Performance". Psychological Science. 12 (5): 385–390. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00371. ISSN 0956-7976. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  20. ^ Hecht, Marvin A.; Ambady, Nalini (1999-9). "Nonverbal communication and psychology: Past and future". New Jersey Journal of Communication. 7 (2): 156–170. doi:10.1080/15456879909367364. ISSN 1067-9154. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  21. ^ Weisbuch, Max; Ambady, Nalini (2009). "Unspoken cultural influence: Exposure to and influence of nonverbal bias". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 96 (6): 1104–1119. doi:10.1037/a0015642. ISSN 1939-1315. PMC 2763368. PMID 19469590.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  22. ^ Gray, Heather M.; Ishii, Keiko; Ambady, Nalini (2011-11). "Misery Loves Company: When Sadness Increases the Desire for Social Connectedness". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 37 (11): 1438–1448. doi:10.1177/0146167211420167. ISSN 0146-1672. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  23. ^ Gray, Heather M.; Ishii, Keiko; Ambady, Nalini (2011-11). "Misery Loves Company: When Sadness Increases the Desire for Social Connectedness". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 37 (11): 1438–1448. doi:10.1177/0146167211420167. ISSN 0146-1672. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  24. ^ Slepian, Michael L.; Bogart, Kathleen R.; Ambady, Nalini (2014-03-28). "Thin-Slice Judgments in the Clinical Context". Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 10 (1): 131–153. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-090413-123522. ISSN 1548-5943.
  25. ^ a b Slepian, Michael L.; Bogart, Kathleen R.; Ambady, Nalini (2014-03-28). "Thin-Slice Judgments in the Clinical Context". Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 10 (1): 131–153. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-090413-123522. ISSN 1548-5943.
  26. ^ Ambady, Nalini; Rosenthal, Robert (1992). "Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 111 (2): 256–274. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.256. ISSN 1939-1455.
  27. ^ a b c d Ambady, Nalini; Rosenthal, Robert (1992). "Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 111 (2): 256–274. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.256. ISSN 1939-1455.
  28. ^ History & Archives: AAAS Prize for Behavioral Science Research



Culture and the brain

Nalini Ambady and Jamshed Bharucha collaborated on a study published in the journal 'Current Directions in Psychological Science' in 2009 that focused on articles regarding how culture can affect neural activation. They suggest a framework for cultural neuroscience in which both features are objective: culture mapping and source analysis.[1] Culture mapping is a research technique that requires mapping cognitive or neural differences across cultures. It also involves regulating which cognitive or neural processes differ across cultures without determining whether differences are learned or innate. [2]

Culture mapping can be in two forms. The first form can show how the environments of different cultures are processed inversely by individuals from a particular culture. The second form illustrates how the same environment is processed differently from distinctive cultures. Ambady included a study conducted by Tang et al (2006) which looked at the brain activity of native English and native Chinese speakers while performing numerical and non-numerical tasks.[3] Their results showed that native English speakers had more activation in the left perisylvian cortices such as Wernickesand Broca's area, which are connected with language. However, Native Chinese speakers demonstrated more activation in a visual premotor association area, which is correlated with visual-spatial processing and functions tied to cognitive functioning during numerical tasks. [2]

Moreover, Ambady discusses source analysis as the process of defining the source or causes of culture mappings. She explains three sources of cultural universals and differences:

Behavioural genetics studies examining the functional role of 5-HTT have recognized individuals carrying the short allele seem prone to higher levels of anxiety and depression compared to long allele carriers. In addition, individuals with the short allele showed greater amygdala activation, during an emotion-matching task relative to individuals with the long allele.[2]

The evidence shows how cultural learning originates from inspecting neural processing in bilinguals and learners of a second language. A study included in the article compared Korean adults who were adopted by French families before the age of 8. The results demonstrate how Korean-born subjects did not remember their native languages and were fluent in French. However, both groups demonstrated parallel patterns of activation to sentences spoken in French, Korean or other foreign languages, implying that proficiency in the language of the new culture was connected with neural changes.[2]

  • Similarity or difference explains the difference in stimuli and pattern structures across cultures.

In a meta-analysis comparison, the neuroimaging results for word reading across different languages and cultures found one common area of dispensation across language and cultures. They proposed the existence of a visual word form area in the left mid fusiform gyrus, which seems to be central to word recognition. They also found culture and language-specific patterns of activation. This was through the example of the Chinese writing system which, for example, requires more activation in the visual areas in both hemispheres than do western alphabets.[2]

Overall, Ambady's study shows the neuroscience of culture is at a promising stage. Recent studies and research in the field of cultural differences are sparser than cultural mapping and source analysis research, but new evidence promises to advance this.

"Thin slices" of behavior

[edit]

In collaboration with Kathleen R. Bogart and Michael L. Slepian, Nalini advanced the study of “thin slices” of behavior when researching the ability to precisely recognize characteristic traits people possess along with their individuality. This method establishes a way to predict an individual’s behavior with something as simple as a swift observation of their nonverbal behavior.

There is only one limitation to this method, a clinician’s analysis can falter when presented with a patient suffering from particular clinical disabilities (e.g. movement disorders such as Parkinson’s disease), since healthy nonverbal behavior is impaired or masked by the exhibited symptoms of these disorders.

With a glimpse of someone’s “thin slices” of behavior, Nalini and her colleagues believed that just by observing nonverbal behavior, one can determine with accuracy to a certain extent, behavioral outcomes influenced by personality disorders (e.g. anxiety, depression etc.); mental states (e.g. suicide); and how an individual adapts to life events. One could even go as far as specific types of life events such as traumatic experience (e.g. divorce, sexual abuse etc.)[4].

Conventional methods already entail clinicians to observe their patients and make diagnoses through intuition based on the patient’s personality traits and nonverbal behavior. With the added method of “thin-slice” judgement, specialists would have another valuable resource to provide more qualitative assessments of their patients.  

Accuracy of prediction

[edit]

Previously, through a meta-analysis published in 1992,  Nalini and co-author Rober Rosenthal coined the term “thin slices” of expressive behavior as a valid method of predicting human behavior. The results of their findings presents that thin slices provide a 70% accuracy prediction as opposed to the 30% when using other assessment methods[5].

They provided three explanations as to why this method is so accurate:

Emotions such as anger, fear or dominance are easily recognized due to how well they are recognized universally and the important role they play in survival and adaptability. In contrast, non-crucial survival emotions such as humor, are more challenging to detect due to the necessity to identify through inferential processes.

  • Truths to stereotypes and their effects on self-fulfilling prophecies.

It is believed that individuals react to stereotype cues, whether they are biological or behavioral. These stereotypes create expectations in others that influence an individual’s behavior toward the target person and/or group. Research has demonstrated how such expectations will affect people’s positive or negative behavior toward targeted individuals. By exhibiting these behavior toward target individuals, they in turn modify their behavior and confirm the previous expectations. This creates an endless loop of self-fulfilling prophecies.

  • Evidence based on the disruptive effect of thinking and reasoning.

A reason why evidence drawn from “thin slices” are accurate is due to the fact that the individuals were not exposed to distracting stimuli. Research found that subjects were less accurate in judgement toward other target individuals during face-to-face scenarios in contrast to impressions evaluated from videotaped targeted individuals. This draws upon two types of stimuli, external and internal.

External stimuli refers to verbal components that can be distracting within actual interactions, and require cognitive resources to process internally. These internal processes can also be distracting because it requires the function of thinking and reasoning which can overpower and cloud judgement.

Thin slices of behavior are "snap" judgments therefore bypass all the distracting stimuli, creating a more accurate impression of the targeted individual(s).


  1. ^ Ambady, Nalini; Bharucha, Jamshed (2009-12). "Culture and the Brain". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 18 (6): 342–345. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01664.x. ISSN 0963-7214. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  2. ^ a b c d e Ambady, Nalini; Bharucha, Jamshed (2009-12). "Culture and the Brain". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 18 (6): 342–345. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01664.x. ISSN 0963-7214. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  3. ^ Tang, Yiyuan; Zhang, Wutian; Chen, Kewei; Feng, Shigang; Ji, Ye; Shen, Junxian; Reiman, Eric M.; Liu, Yijun (2006). "Arithmetic Processing in the Brain Shaped by Cultures". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 103 (28): 10775–10780. ISSN 0027-8424.
  4. ^ Slepian, Michael L.; Bogart, Kathleen R.; Ambady, Nalini (2014-03-28). "Thin-Slice Judgments in the Clinical Context". Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 10 (1): 131–153. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-090413-123522. ISSN 1548-5943.
  5. ^ Ambady, Nalini; Rosenthal, Robert (1992). "Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 111 (2): 256–274. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.256. ISSN 1939-1455.